THERE is a widespread
assumption today that faith is largely a question of intellectual
belief. The debate around science and religion, for example,
proceeds on the assumption that both activities deliver facts and
theories that can, or cannot, coexist. But what if faith is not
primarily about facts and theories? They may come, but only after
time spent exploring a way of life, committing to a practice.
Alternatively, think of
mission. Books, popular and scholarly, are written to assemble
evidence designed to persuade non-believers that Jesus lived, and
that the resurrection happened. Or proofs for the existence of God
are strained again as if they might rationally convince the
sceptics. But, surely, most people are more likely to find faith
because, one day, perhaps by mistake, they walked into a holy
place, or were helped by a Good Samaritan, or were caught by a
visceral experience of love or loss. Thinking about what it means
is vital, but belief rests on, and stays alive because of, the
embodied experience.
A NEW area in science
suggests that these intuitions are right. It is known as "embodied
cognition", which roughly translates as: what we think, feel, and
do depends not only on the brain, but also on the body. We are not
brains in vats. "It is not the brain alone that gives rise to
consciousness. Consciousness is grounded or contextualised in the
body," the psychologist Canon Fraser Watts explained at a recent
conference on embodied cognition and theology, organised by the
International Society for Science and Religion.
Think of a footballer
kicking the ball. He does not perform mathematical calculations in
his head to land it in the back of the net. He feels his way
through the kick, and hence fans say that he has a good feel for
the game. Alternatively, recall how you involuntarily move as you
speak. It turns out that the movement is not incidental. It
actually helps you to reach for the right word, rehearse the
argument, and remember details. Try speaking with your hands tied
behind your back.
Further, language itself
reminds us of the importance of the body. We say "on the one hand"
and "on the other hand" when explaining something, and it is
possible to demonstrate that this is no mere idiom. It makes
complicated arguments easier to understand. Or, again, you could
say that Marcel Proust was right: the smell of the madeleine was
crucial to his capacity for recollection.
THE part played by the
body takes centre stage in newer ideas about the origins of
religion, too. The story here is one of mimesis, or imitation, the
American sociologist Robert Bellah suggests in his book
Religion in Human Evolution. Animals communicate with
their bodies in their behaviour, he notes: bright plumage is
flaunted, lips curl back in aggression, a head lowers in
submission.
But some animals have a
degree of freedom around the edges of this otherwise mechanical
display. It is not done automatically, but can be played with,
perhaps reflecting the creature's own character. It is something
that any keen naturalist will spot. When filming Life in the
Undergrowth, David Attenborough reported how he had to stop
thinking of spiders as "mechanical little creatures". He reported,
"some are lazy, some are hard-working, some don't like light. They
all have it."
This mix of strict
imitation and personal play is vital in the story of religion,
Bellah says, because it is the precursor of ritual. When ritual
speaks to an individual, it is not automatic, although it will look
similar to when it was last performed. Instead, the individual
makes it his or her own. It is only when both dynamics are
operative that it can convey a feeling or express an understanding.
Lighting a candle; bowing in prayer; raising a hand in praise -
these embodied gestures are not spoken, they are done, and they are
all the more powerful for it. Man can embody truth, W. B. Yeats
reflected, when he cannot rationally know it.
The psychologist Abraham
Maslow tells how he became converted to the value of rituals,
having previously thought them "silly", when one day he was taking
part in a colourful academic procession. He suddenly saw, in his
mind's eye, a line reaching back into the past and stretching into
the future. It spoke of the deep meaning of university life. It
made sense when he allowed the embodied reality of the symbolism to
speak to him.
ALLOWING such rituals and
actions, religious practices and experiences, to speak of truth
seems to be the problem for people today. Our culture has developed
in such a way that it is inclined spontaneously to rank what can be
calculated in the mind over other ways of knowing. But this may be
changing. The heart has its reasons of which reason knows nothing,
Pascal mused. The new science is suggesting that he was right. As
the poet Yves Bonnefoy wittily put it: excarnation is simply
"wrong-headed religion".
Mark Vernon is the author of Love: All that matters
(Hodder Education, £7.99 (CT Bookshop £7.20);
978-1-44415-675-1)